Learn R Programming

psychTools (version 2.4.3)

holzinger.swineford: The raw and transformed data from Holzinger and Swineford, 1939

Description

A classic data set in psychometrics is that from Holzinger and Swineford (1939). A 4 and 5 factor solution to 24 of these variables problem is presented by Harman (1976), and 9 of these are used by the lavaan package. The two data sets were supplied by Keith Widaman.

Usage

data(holzinger.swineford)
     data(holzinger.raw)
     data(holzinger.dictionary)

Arguments

Format

A data frame with 301 observations on the following 33 variables. Longer descriptions taken from Thompson, (1998).

case

a numeric vector

school

School Pasteur or Grant-White

grade

Grade (7 or 8)

female

male = 1, female = 2

ageyr

age in years

mo

months over year

agemo

Age in months

t01_visperc

Visual perception test from Spearman VPT Part I

t02_cubes

Cubes, Simplification of Brighams Spatial Relations Test

t03_frmbord

Paper formboard-Shapes that can be combined to form a target

t04_lozenges

Lozenges from Thorndike-Shapes flipped over then identify target

t05_geninfo

General Information Verbal Test

t06_paracomp

Paragraph Comprehension Test

t07_sentcomp

Sentence Completion Test

t08_wordclas

Word clasification-Which word not belong in set

t09_wordmean

Word Meaning Test

t10_addition

Speeded addition test

t11_code

Speeded codetest-Transform shapes into alpha with code

t12_countdot

Speeded counting of dots in shap

t13_sccaps

Speeded discrimation of straight and curved caps

t14_wordrecg

Memory of Target Words

t15_numbrecg

Memory of Target Numbers

t16_figrrecg

Memory of Target Shapes

t17_objnumb

Memory of object-Number association targets

t18_numbfig

Memory of number-Object association targets

t19_figword

Memory of figure-Word association target

t20_deduction

Deductive Math Ability

t21_numbpuzz

Math number puzzles

t22_probreas

Math word problem reasoning

t23_series

Completion of a Math Number Series

t24_woody

Woody-McCall mixed math fundamentals test

t25_frmbord2

Revision of t3-Paper form board

t26_flags

Flags-possible substitute for t4 lozenges

Details

The following commentary was provided by Keith Widaman:

``The Holzinger and Swineford (1939) data have been used as a model data set by many investigators. For example, Harman (1976) used the ``24 Psychological Variables" example prominently in his authoritative text on multiple factor analysis, and the data presented under this rubric consisted of 24 of the variables from the Grant-White school (N = 145). Meredith (1964a, 1964b) used several variables from the Holzinger and Swineford study in his work on factorial invariance under selection. Joreskog (1971) based his work on multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis using the Holzinger and Swineford data, subsetting the data into four groups.

Rosseel, who developed the `lavaan' package for R, included 9 of the manifest variables from Holzinger and Swineford (1939) as a ``resident" data set when one downloads the `lavaan' package. Several background variables are included in this ``resident" data set in addition to 9 of the psychological tests (which are named x1 -- x9 in the data set). When analyzing these data, I found the distributions of the variables (means, SDs) did not match the sample statistics from the original article. For example, in the ``resident" data set in `lavaan', scores on all manifest variables ranged between 0 and 10, sample means varied between 3 and 6, and sample SDs varied between 1.0 and 1.5. In the original data set, scores ranges were rather different across tests, with some variables having scores that ranged between 0 and 20, but other manifest variables having scores ranging from 50 to over 300 -- with obvious attendant differences in sample means and SDs.

After a bit of snooping (i.e., data analysis), I discovered that the 9 variables in the ``resident" data set in `lavaan' had been rescored through ratio transformations. The ratio transformations involved dividing the raw score for each person on a given test by a particular constant for that test that transformed scores on the test to have the desired range.

I decided to perform transformations of all 26 variables so that two data sets could be available to interested researchers:"

holzinger.raw are the raws scores on all variables from Holzinger & Swineford (1939)

holzinger.swineford are rescaled scores on all variables from Holzinger & Swineford.

holzinger.dictionary is a list of the variable names in short and long form.

... Widaman continues:

``As several persons have noted, Harman (1976) used data only from the Grant-White school (N = 145) for his 24 Psychological Variables data set. In doing so, Harman replaced t03_frmbord and t04_lozenges with t25_frmbord2 and t26_flags, because the latter two tests were experimental tests that were designed to be more appropriate for this age level. This substitution is fine, as long as one analyzes data from only the Grant- White school. If one wishes to perform multiple-group analyses and uses school as a grouping variable (as Meredith, 1964a, 1964b, and Joreskog, 1971, did), then tests 25 and 26 should not be used."

``As have others, Gorsuch (1983) mentioned that analyses based on the raw data reported by Holzinger and Swineford (1939) will not produce statistics (means, SDs, correlations) that match precisely the values reported by Holzinger and Swineford or Harman (1976). Following Gorsuch, I have assumed that the raw data are correct. Applying factor analytic techniques to the raw data from the Grant-White school and to the summary data reported by Harman (1976) will produce slightly different results, but results that differ in only minor, unimportant details."

These data are interesting not just for the historical completeness of having the original data, but also as an example of suppressor variables. Age and grade are positively correlated, and scores are higher in the 8th grade than in the 7th grade. But age (particularly in months) is negatively correlated with many of the cognitive tasks, and when grade and age are both entered into regression, this negative correlation is enhanced. That is, although increasing grade increases cognitive performance, younger children in both grades do better than the older children.

References

Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Harman, Harry Horace (1967), Modern factor analysis. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.

Holzinger, K. J., & Swineford, F. (1939). A study in factor analysis: The stability of a bi-factor solution. Supplementary Educational Monographs, no. 48. Chicago: University of Chicago, Department of Education.

Joreskog, K. G. (1971). Simultaneous factor analysis in several populations. Psychometrika, 36, 409-426.

Meredith, W. (1964a). Notes on factorial invariance. Psychometrika, 29, 177-185.

Meredith, W. (1964b). Rotation to achieve factorial invariance. Psychometrika, 29, 177-206.

Meredith, W. (1977). On weighted Procrustes and hyperplane fitting in factor analytic rotation. Psychometrika, 42, 491-522.

Thompson, Bruce. Five Methodology Errors in Educational Research:The Pantheon of Statistical Significance and Other Faux Pas. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association(San Diego, CA, April 13-17,1998)

See Also

psych::Holzinger

Examples

Run this code
data(holzinger.raw)
psych::describe(holzinger.raw)
data(holzinger.dictionary)
holzinger.dictionary  #to see the longer names for these data (taken from Thompson)

#Compare these to the lavaan correlation matrix
psych::lowerCor(holzinger.swineford[ 7+ c(1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 9, 10, 12,  13)])

psych::lmCor(t01_visperc + t05_geninfo + t08_wordclas ~ grade + agemo,data = holzinger.raw)
psych::lmCor( t06_paracomp ~ grade + agemo, data=holzinger.swineford)
psych::mediate(t06_paracomp  ~ grade + (agemo),data = holzinger.raw,std=TRUE)

#show the omega structure of the 24 variables
 om4 <- psych::omega(holzinger.swineford[8:31],4)
psych::omega.diagram(om4,sl=FALSE,main="26 variables from Holzinger-Swineford")

#these data also show an interesting suppression effect

psych::lowerCor(holzinger.swineford[c(3,7,12:14)])
psych::lmCor( t06_paracomp ~ grade + agemo, data=holzinger.swineford)
#or show as a mediation effect
mod <- psych::mediate(t06_paracomp  ~ grade + (agemo),data = holzinger.raw,std=TRUE,n.iter=50)
summary(mod)

#now, show a plot of these effets
plot(t07_sentcomp ~ agemo, col=c("red","blue")[holzinger.swineford$grade -6],
  pch=26-holzinger.swineford$grade,data=holzinger.swineford,
   ylab="Sentence Comprehension",xlab="Age in Months",
   main="Sentence Comprehension varies by age and grade")
   #we use lmCor to figure out the lines 
   #note that we need to not plot the default graph
by(holzinger.swineford,holzinger.swineford$grade -6,function(x) abline(
     psych::lmCor(t07_sentcomp ~ agemo, data=x, std=FALSE, plot=FALSE), 
     lty=c("dashed","solid")[x$grade-6]))
text(190,3.3,"grade = 8")
text(190,2,"grade = 7") 

Run the code above in your browser using DataLab